Volume 20, Number 2, June 2014
Copyright © 2014 Society for Music Theory
Making MusicDora A. Hanninen
|
[2.1] John Cage might be described as a “maker of music.” This characterization downplays his personal agency as “composer” and suggests an affinity with craftsmen and creators of traditional Buddhist art, such as sand mandala and thangka paintings—people who make art, but often according to a prescribed plan, within precise constraints, with a sense of discipline directed beyond the artistic result.(1) Recasting Cage from “composer” to a “maker” of music also allies him with performers and listeners, in a blurring or plurality of roles that Cage himself recognized, and played. Whereas Cage provides the impetus that brings a piece of music into being, the performer makes the score into sound. The performer’s role is an active one: the performer does something, inevitably adds something, to what is notated. Listeners also “make music,” and so too music analysts. To listen to or analyze music is to enter a sound world with full attention, in a spirit of active engagement. Listening and analysis are not merely receptive, a matter of accurate perception and representation. They are also creative: we make music, through acts of apperception and interpretation. Drawing on the ethic of analysis articulated in “Asking Questions,” this paper explores how two pianists and one reflective listener-analyst (me) “make music” of the first system from the sixth piece of Cage’s Etudes Australes (1974), in two recordings by Grete Sultan (1978) and Sabine Liebner (2011).
[2.2] In his visual art, Cage uses chance to create “improbable” compositions—prints, drawings, and watercolors in which proportions, placement, shape, weight, and color operate independently of one another and outside conventional compositional principles.(2) In the Etudes Australes, star charts introduce the element of chance into music composition.(3)
Of course, the distribution of stars represented in the Atlas Australis is not actually random; it is governed by the laws of physics. But the mapping of points from three-dimensional space onto a plane, then points in a plane to musical pitches (different sorts of objects with radically different principles for interaction), all filtered through and altered by the proclivities of human perception and cognition, is essentially random.(4) “Random” does not mean “uniform,” however. The “tables of random numbers” to which Cage refers approach an even distribution at the scale of infinity. But music perception is local. Psychologists estimate the size of the window of working memory at 2–3 seconds and say that we tend to chunk events into groups of “seven, plus or minus two.”(5) Heard through this moving window of the perceptual present, “randomness” in music becomes a font of immense diversity; chance, infinitely, and improbably, creative—especially when we recognize that we tend to hear musical sounds not in isolation but in context, as shaped by one another and by their unique, serendipitous, circumstances. [2.3] The use of chance also has an important psychological effect. The challenge it poses for the listener, and the music analyst, is not too little information, but too much. As chance tends to minimize repetition, it thwarts our ability to chunk, categorize, predict, and remember; it stymies our use of language, from basic semantics (attaching labels to things) through to complex description. Instead of focusing on what we cannot do in this environment, I’m more interested in what the music, in its superabundant particularity, does do, what it makes us do. Information overload has a way of forcing us into the present, where with muted memory we remain, taking in each new sound as it touches, then enters, awareness. To hear in this way is to be in a state of music-consciousness; it is to experience listening in its manner of operation.
[2.4] In “Asking Questions,” I developed an ethic for the analysis of Cage’s music around three ideas: that analysis be nonreductive; it be based on open (relatively non-intentional) listening; and it emphasize process over product. Music analysis tends toward reduction to the extent that it assumes a prescribed repertoire of sound-objects, conflates repetitions as if they were interchangeable, and surrenders the particularity of individual sounds to their place in a group. In contrast, nonreductive analysis construes musical sounds as fundamentally interactive: “sounds” become inseparable from “relations,” shaped by their influence on one another. Open listening indicates a state of aural engagement, of acute and directed attention (approximately “intentionality” in a philosopher’s sense), largely without expectation.(6) It is a kind of listening that largely escapes the well-worn habits I call “pre-emptive perception” (of things or relations) and “pre-perception” (off-the-shelf interpretations, often shaped by particular music theories). When we listen in this way, we indicate a willingness to re-open fundamental questions about the nature of musical sound. What is a musical “sound”? Is it a note? A note soaked in resonance? Two or more notes? Must these be struck at the same time, or just close together? How far can one sound reach into other sounds just heard, heard with, or to come? When is a sound fully formed? And where is a sound? Can we point to it in a score? Or are sounds more diffuse sorts of things, dispersed in time and the web of contexts that embed them? Once we stop listening for our own expectations, we become more attuned to the strange richness of musical experience. Listening becomes a state of awareness poised in not knowing. The nature of analysis shifts, from an attempt to describe or define music as something outside of ourselves to a more personal and interactive exploration of one’s own hearing. [2.5] If we shift the focus from analytic product to process, how might we proceed for a piece like Etudes Australes VI, composed with chance? What do we do? I cannot say what one should do; only describe, what I did do, more or less, with multiple changes of course. Recognizing the indeterminacy of the score with respect to dynamics and articulation, I began with an exercise in observation: I created an annotated score for Sultan’s (and Liebner’s) performance, in which I recorded aspects of dynamics, articulation, resonance, and timing, as well as a few gestures. As I worked, I noticed a certain consistency in the way I heard specific notes as colored or refracted by one another, and moments as having certain qualities—qualities that seemed to sit just beyond my horizon of verbalization. [2.6] To get some of my impressions into a form I could work with, I developed an aural score for the first system of Sultan’s performance in which I copied some markings from the annotated score, but tried to clarify, through my imperfect attempts at representation, my own evolving sense of what I heard. I noted salient repetitions, transfers, and voice-leading in pitch or pitch-class. I marked gestural or other groupings and tried to put qualities into words. These could be individual words or short phrases, nouns or adjectives, verbs or adverbs. But many turned out to be verbs: “absorb,” “accrue,” “adjust,” “capture,” “counter,” “extend,” “fracture,” “grate,” “nestle,” “project,” “revive,” “strike.” My only concern was that the language be concise and apt to my experience.(7) The point was not to assign labels, nor to connect words or sounds into a narrative, but to frame analytical questions: What is the impression I have, and what might have given rise to it? What am I hearing subconsciously? What else might I hear, if I transformed subconscious impressions into conscious ones, and then listened again? [2.7] The aural score was a working document. A memory trace and visual aid, it helped me to stabilize some aspects of my hearing just enough to probe my impressions further. I began to ask different questions, about the constitution and formative context for individual sounds. I examined the score’s raw potential with respect to voice leading in pitch and pitch-class, and pitch-class distribution, then brought this information into dialogue with my impressions, using it to “key out” some of the musical contingencies at work, much as one might “key out” an unfamiliar mushroom with a series of questions about its appearance and environment.(8) As I did this, the metaphoric language seemed to reposition itself in my thinking, to become a sort of shorthand for an analytical statement about the complex relations between a sound and its musical context. I then took my hearing in the direction of comparative analysis, repeating the process for the first system of Liebner’s performance. [2.8] To summarize, then, analysis proceeded in roughly four stages: listening; limited transcription (in an annotated score, of dynamics, articulation, etc.); interpretation (in an aural score, of pitch connections, groupings, actions, and verbalizations); and comparative analysis, both against the raw potential of the score (pitch-class voice leading and distributions) and across performances. The process involved a constant cycling and exchange among three kinds of activities: (1) an attempt to identify and partially stabilize impressions; (2) a redirection of attention, which prompted new observations; and (3) reinterpretation and continual transformation of my hearing.
[2.9] We begin as a listener does, with some music: in this case, two recordings of the same notated passage from the opening of Etude VI, in performances first by Grete Sultan, then by Sabine Liebner (Audio Examples 1a and 1b). Slide 9 provides a score for the first two systems of Etude VI. Cage thought of the Etudes Australes as a duet for piano solo, notated on two grand staffs, one for the right hand and one for the left. The score is determinate with respect to pitch (in staff notation) and rhythm (proportional notation), and indeterminate with respect to tempo, dynamics, articulation, and gestural shaping. With the right hand reaching down to A2 (a tenth below middle C) and the left hand up to C6 (two octaves above it), hand crossings are frequent.
Audio Example 1a. Opening of Etude VI performed by Grete Sultan Audio Example 1b. Opening of Etude VI performed by Sabine Liebner [2.10] Before we proceed to analysis, I need to say a few things about Cage’s pitch notation and how it relates to the music one hears. Cage uses three types of noteheads. Diamond noteheads (in the low bass at the very start of each etude) indicate keys depressed by rubber wedges throughout (e.g., in Etude VI, D1, F1, and C2); these generate sympathetic resonance as other notes are struck. I call this “permanent sympathetic resonance,” not because the resonance is always the same (on the contrary, its composition in frequency and amplitude is in constant flux), but because the potential for resonance remains constant. Open noteheads indicate pitches that are to be sustained by finger pedal as long as physically possible, that is, until the hand must move to reach another note; in most cases, Cage uses horizontal “pedal” brackets to show the actual duration. Pitches written in open noteheads also generate sympathetic resonance as black noteheads or other white noteheads are struck; I call this “temporary resonance.” Finally, black noteheads are generally played short. They do not, themselves, provide resonance, but excite most of the sympathetic resonance associated with upper partials of diamond or open noteheads. Note that sympathetic resonance can introduce shadow pitches, with no notational correlate. For example, coincidence between the fifth partial of a struck note (black or open notehead) and the sixth partial of a sustained one (open or diamond notehead) can generate a shadow pitch in the third octave above the fundamentals (striking C2, with [2.11] The sympathetic haze is an integral part of the listener’s experience of the Etudes Australes, but it eludes standard musical notation. Overtone trails and shadow notes emerge, speak, and retreat below the threshold of audibility; at times there is only a hazy backdrop of inharmonic vibration. Pitches separated by white space in the score sound not only in relation to one another but within a shadow world of resonance where individual partials rise and evolve at different times, at different amplitudes, in response to the performer’s tempo, dynamics (strike speed), details of timing, register (string thickness), acoustics of the piano, and microphone placement. Whether or not one focuses on the sympathetic haze, one listens through it, as a spectral environment that envelops and colors the sound of every new note. [2.12] The care with which Sultan prepared her performance of the Etudes is apparent from her marked score (see Slide 10).(9) Although Cage’s proportional notation for rhythm is precise, it is hard to read, requiring very fine judgments in spatial position, often between notes too far apart on the page to grasp in a single visual fixation. To improve visual clarity, Sultan attaches a long stem to each note. The stems settle questions of simultaneity, succession, and precedence; they also better convey a sense of attack rhythm, which in turn inspires interpretive decisions for physical and musical gestures.(10) These are represented in Sultan’s score as patterns of arsis and thesis, slurs, arrows, and hand choreography (fingerings, and hand crossings with indications of “over” and “under”). Here and there throughout the etudes she adds character descriptions, such as “floating,” “clear,” and “birds.” Slurs suggest groupings of notes into constellations or audible gestures. Lines in red pencil trace a number of “pitch-class channels” as a note is repeated or a pc travels from one octave to another. Interestingly, her representation of these pitch-class channels is selective: accounting for only sixty percent of those available, she omits some of the most audible connections, but shows some of the most obscure.(11) Did she think of these pitch-class channels as lines of musical continuity, or were they a mnemonic device? Whatever they meant to her, what might we, as listeners and analysts, make of them?
[2.13] Slide 11 shows the first two systems of my annotated score for Sultan’s performance. Working first from the recording, I later compared my draft with her performance score and added some of her markings. Six colors (red, orange, yellow, green, blue, violet) represent six levels of loudness from fortissimo to pianissimo. (There is no red, for ff, in this excerpt.) Dynamics enclosed in boxes are Sultan’s, as are all brown markings: these include some articulations, gestures (arsis and thesis units), words (which here indicate dynamics or aspects of rhythm), and numerals at the end of each system (reflecting Cage’s proportional notation, these indicate, in minutes and seconds, Sultan’s projected timings for the end of each system). In deep red, I’ve added some articulations taken from the recording, as well as breaks between “phrases” and the first set of timings, which indicate actual timings in Sultan’s 1978 recording. Connecting noteheads, solid magenta lines highlight some pc channels; solid cyan (bright light blue) lines, show strong pitch voice-leading. Brown slurs or lines represent a few gestures formed by succession or connection.
[2.14] Slide 12 is my aural score for the first system of Sultan’s performance. I’ve rearranged Cage’s notation in a more traditional layout of four staves distinguished by register: 8va treble, treble, bass, 8vb bass. The rearranged score better represents aural stream segregation by register and so is easier to follow. While I’ve reproduced a few markings for dynamics and articulations, for the most part the aural score focuses on different kinds of things—not facts about the performance but aspects of interpretation, such as voice-leading, harmonies, musical segments, and local repetitions. Brown text conveys my impressions of certain moments; black text uses standard technical language to make analytic points. Magenta lines connect pitch repetitions within the same register; maroon lines, pitch-class repetitions in different registers.(12) Cyan lines highlight pitch voice leading by semitone, within register.
[2.15] Sultan’s performance begins with a cat’s leap from a broken chord in the high register down to middle C (C4). Even a soft landing in this spot is enough to excite permanent resonance from C2 (shown by a gray shaded horizontal bar), which is enhanced by Sultan’s anapestic gesture to a marked attack. From C4 as a base, the minor tenth C4– [2.16] A loud interjection from a whole-tone tetrachord ({FGAB}) opens up the low register; strong contrast between this whole-tone harmony and the prevailing chromatic counterpoint creates a sense of “spread.” But activity in the main register quickly resumes: the falling fifth < [2.17] Over the course of the first system in Sultan’s performance, then, I hear many lines of local continuity, and one strong break just before the chromatic trichord { [2.18] It was then that I started to ask different questions, to look more closely at pitch-class channels, voice leading, distributions, and their lines of potential continuity and disjunction within the first system—at force fields created by repetition and voice leading in pitch and pitch-class, that are implicit with the score but may, or may not, be projected or heard in a given (or any) performance. [2.19] A comprehensive study of the score’s raw potential for pitch and pitch-class voice leading and comparison with my own hearing proved instructive. Slide 13 uses a combination of color coding and solid versus dashed lines to highlight repetitions and voice leading in pitch and pitch-class. Solid raspberry lines indicate the strongest relation: a pitch repeated, in register. Solid maroon lines connect a pitch class with its repetition in a different octave. Together these constitute the complete network of pc channels described earlier.(13) Cyan (bright blue) lines show pitch voice leading by semitone. Solid dark blue lines represent pitch-class voice leading by 9 or 11 semitones (i.e., a semitone displaced by one octave).(14) Dashed dark blue lines indicate the weakest type of connection: pc motions by interval class 1 that are displaced by two or more octaves. The strength of the force field of repetitions and voice leading that a listener perceives at any given moment depends on details of performance, as well as each listener’s proclivities and focus of attention. Dynamics, timing, and articulation can bring notes into contact with one another or force them apart, perhaps outside the window of working memory. An interesting aspect of this representation is the way notes seem to group into constellations: some parts of the piece look tighter than others. Significantly, the nexus of relatively strong connections in pitch and pitch-class that bind much of the first system into force fields largely dissolves around the rift. From the “open” trichord through the “paired fifths” stretch, pitch repetitions, pc octave transfers, and semitone voice leading create a fairly tight weave. But only a few tenuous threads reach across the rift, three by pitch-class voice leading with octave displacement (D3–
[2.20] Continuing along these lines, I then mapped out the distribution of the twelve pitch-classes throughout Etude VI, assigning each pc a color swatch: beige for C (pc 0), chocolate brown for
[2.21] It’s interesting to reflect on the fact that so many lovely connections arise by chance in this first system. Or do they? As a performer, Sultan “makes music” from Cage’s notation, through a series of choices for dynamics, articulation, and gesture. Her choices in turn shaped my hearing, the way I “made music” of the passage, strengthening some lines of pitch or pitch-class voice leading over others’. I assume that each of us will notice different things and express our impressions in different words. The point is not to assert my hearing over others but to characterize it well enough to be able to share it. I expect that we will find some common ground in basic principles for music perception, including pitch repetition, octave equivalence, pitch proximity, pitch-class voice leading, grouping based on temporal proximity, and chunking supported by repetition. Each performer also makes his or her own choices: these can encourage us to hear the “same” passage differently—to make different music from the same notes. [2.22] To date, there are four commercial recordings of Book I of the Etudes, by Grete Sultan, Stephen Drury, Steffen Schleiermacher, and Sabine Liebner. While all eight etudes in Book I (and 32 etudes in Books I–IV) have the same notated length (eight systems on a two-page spread, four systems per page), in accordance with the degree of freedom Cage’s indeterminate notation affords, the four performances differ in tempo and overall character, as well as in numerous details of dynamics, articulations, timing, and gesture. Slide 15 graphs the length of each of the eight etudes in Book I across these four recordings. Length is in seconds; the slower the tempo, the higher the point on the graph. Sultan, Drury, and Liebner choose tempi that are fairly consistent from one etude to another. Schleiermacher’s tempi vary the most; Liebner’s performance is consistently the slowest, roughly half the speed of Sultan’s and Drury’s. Each performance is a unique encounter among the score, a pianist, a specific piano, and a recording engineer. Liebner balances a much slower tempo with a tendency toward louder dynamics and sharper attacks. Her slow tempo tends to weaken voice leading, as notes move beyond one another’s reach. But it also gives the piano’s sympathetic resonance time to speak and decay, and the listener time to hear it. Whereas in Sultan’s performance most audible interactions are between two notated pitches, in Liebner’s performance notated pitches often interact with sound shadows—overtone trails, shadow pitches, and the sympathetic haze. While Sultan also makes good use of resonance and Liebner of voice leading within and across registers, the two performances create different sound worlds, with implications for the listener and analyst.
[2.23] Slide 16 provides an annotated score for Liebner’s performance of the first two systems. Five colors again represent the spectrum of dynamics from red (ff) to light blue (p). (No violet, for pp, appears in this excerpt.) All articulations (maroon) are based on the recording. With sympathetic resonance so prominent throughout Liebner’s recording, I’ve added a number of brown bars to represent strong overtones; these “overtone trails” are only approximate in length and do not represent decay (nor are they comprehensive). Slide 17 provides an aural score for the first system. Now using black for sympathetic resonance, shading suggests the gradual decay in amplitude. The piece begins with a fortissimo “strike!” followed by a five-second fade. A strong overtone trail from C4 reaches past and absorbs the C6 that begins a “continuation” gesture. The right hand dives to G4, which also leaves a strong trail; at this slow tempo, the last note of the “continuation” figure, A6, does as well. Five seconds pass before a deliberate stomp in the bass from
[2.24] I hear this high [2.25] Coming in quick succession, the three forte notes <A4, D4, A2> sound like a confirmation, an exclamation point. But the punctuation seems unwarranted: drawn in by the overtone trails on A4, [2.26] Reintroducing a register long dormant, the next note, A5, easily reaches up to D6, a perfect fourth away. The low “open” E that comes in between may be from a different world, but it also contributes to the predominantly diatonic backdrop that accommodates and absorbs the entrance of G3 as a sort of lower fifth to D6. The move from D to G is slow, taking more than five seconds. Its compressed tritone transpose, <
[SLIDE 19] [2.27] Liebner’s choice of a slower tempo, often coordinated with choices for dynamics, attack, and details of timing, lead me to hear this passage differently than in Sultan’s recording. There are three main differences. First, I hear Sultan’s performance as dividing into three parts, demarcated first by the high [2.28] At this point, one might wish to revisit the two performances of the opening passage one more time, in sequence, first by Sultan (Sound Example 2a), then by Liebner (Sound Example 2b). Audio Example 2a. Opening of Etude VI performed by Grete Sultan (first system) Audio Example 2b. Opening of Etude VI performed by Sabine Liebner (first system)
[2.29] It is well known that Cage didn’t like recordings; he found them too much the same, a flattening of musical experience. While I understand this point of view, and agree that there is an important difference between the fixity of text in a recording and the variability and unpredictability of live performance, I don’t think that listening to a recording necessarily reduces our multiple encounters with a piece to a single experience, repeatable in all its detail. Instead of finding each hearing of Sultan’s, or Liebner’s, recording to be the same, I continually noticed new things and heard other things differently.(20) My hearing of these passages was, and remains, in process. The attempt to fix some aspects of my hearing transforms others. Listening, and analysis, move in a dance of attending and being led “astray,” of fixing and pointing, of setting out to answer, but ending up asking, questions. In the Etudes Australes Cage’s use of chance creates a surface of crystalline particularity. There is always something to hear. With no path laid out by the composer, the listener cannot become complacent. We must remain engaged; we are each responsible for, to an extent make, the music in our own ears. If Cage’s intention in using chance was to explore unthought-of possibilities, to create unique musical situations, I found that concerted hearing helped me to explore and enter into these more deeply. Must the input to consciousness be “new,” must it be unprecedented, to have a new experience? Or is our experience inevitably new (“no such thing as repetition”) if we enter into it, if we open our eyes, our ears, our minds, enough?
[SLIDE 22]
Dora A. Hanninen Appendix: Image Sources
Works CitedBabbitt, Milton. [1972] 2003. “Contemporary Music Composition and Music Theory as Contemporary Intellectual History.” In Collected Essays of Milton Babbitt, edited by Stephen Peles, with Stephen Dembski, Andrew Mead, and Joseph N. Straus, 270–307. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Babbitt, Milton. [1972] 2003. “Contemporary Music Composition and Music Theory as Contemporary Intellectual History.” In Collected Essays of Milton Babbitt, edited by Stephen Peles, with Stephen Dembski, Andrew Mead, and Joseph N. Straus, 270–307. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Brown, Kathan. 2001. John Cage Visual Art: To Sober and Quiet the Mind. San Francisco: Crown Point Press. Brown, Kathan. 2001. John Cage Visual Art: To Sober and Quiet the Mind. San Francisco: Crown Point Press. Brown, Kathan. 2002. “Visual Art.” In The Cambridge Companion to John Cage, edited by David Nicholls, 109–27. New York: Cambridge University Press. —————. 2002. “Visual Art.” In The Cambridge Companion to John Cage, edited by David Nicholls, 109–27. New York: Cambridge University Press. Burns, Edward M. 1999. “Intervals, Scales, and Tuning.” In Psychology of Music, 2nd ed., edited by Diana Deutsch, 215–64. New York: Academic Press. Burns, Edward M. 1999. “Intervals, Scales, and Tuning.” In Psychology of Music, 2nd ed., edited by Diana Deutsch, 215–64. New York: Academic Press. Cage, John. 1961. Silence. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press. Cage, John. 1961. Silence. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press. Cage, John. 1963. A Year From Monday. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press. —————. 1963. A Year From Monday. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press. Cage, John. 2000. John Cage, Writer. Selected and introduced by Richard Kostelanetz. New York: Limelight Editions. —————. 2000. John Cage, Writer. Selected and introduced by Richard Kostelanetz. New York: Limelight Editions. Cage, John. 2010. Every Day is a Good Day: The Visual Art of John Cage. New York: Hayward Publishing. —————. 2010. Every Day is a Good Day: The Visual Art of John Cage. New York: Hayward Publishing. “Cage on Sultan.” 2012. Posted on Cage 100, website to accompany the Cage 100 Exhibition. Special Collections, Performing Arts Library, University of Maryland Libraries. http://lib.guides.umd.edu/content.php?pid=345022&sid=2834657 (accessed October 9, 2012). “Cage on Sultan.” 2012. Posted on Cage 100, website to accompany the Cage 100 Exhibition. Special Collections, Performing Arts Library, University of Maryland Libraries. http://lib.guides.umd.edu/content.php?pid=345022&sid=2834657 (accessed October 9, 2012). Charles, Daniel, ed. 1976. For the Birds: John Cage in Conversation with Daniel Charles. Boston: Marion Boyars. Charles, Daniel, ed. 1976. For the Birds: John Cage in Conversation with Daniel Charles. Boston: Marion Boyars. Guck, Marion A. 1996. “Rehabilitating the Incorrigible.” In Theory, Analysis, and Meaning in Music, edited by Anthony Pople. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Guck, Marion A. 1996. “Rehabilitating the Incorrigible.” In Theory, Analysis, and Meaning in Music, edited by Anthony Pople. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Guck, Marion A. 1997. “Rigors of Subjectivity.” Perspectives of New Music 35, no. 2: 53–64. —————. 1997. “Rigors of Subjectivity.” Perspectives of New Music 35, no. 2: 53–64. Hanninen, Dora A. 2003. “A Theory of Recontextualization in Music: Analyzing Phenomenal Transformations of Repetition.” Music Theory Spectrum 25, no. 1: 59–97. Hanninen, Dora A. 2003. “A Theory of Recontextualization in Music: Analyzing Phenomenal Transformations of Repetition.” Music Theory Spectrum 25, no. 1: 59–97. Hanninen, Dora A. 2004a. “Associative Sets, Categories, and Music Analysis.” Journal of Music Theory 48, no. 2: 147–218. —————. 2004a. “Associative Sets, Categories, and Music Analysis.” Journal of Music Theory 48, no. 2: 147–218. Hanninen, Dora A. 2004b. “Feldman, Analysis, Experience.” Twentieth-Century Music 1, no. 2: 225–251. —————. 2004b. “Feldman, Analysis, Experience.” Twentieth-Century Music 1, no. 2: 225–251. Hanninen, Dora A. 2012. A Theory of Music Analysis: On Segmentation and Associative Organization. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. —————. 2012. A Theory of Music Analysis: On Segmentation and Associative Organization. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. |